The “Uncanny Valley”
Posted by Mark Allen on 10 Apr 2007 | Tagged as: Random Posts
The “Uncanny Valley” describes an area of human perception where something is perceived as familiar, yet contains enough unfamiliar, unresolvable characteristics to also be classified as foreign – and the back-and-forth one’s mind performs as it ping-pongs between the two categorizations of what is being seen, produces an uneasy feeling. This applies particularly to the identification of other living things with identifiable characteristics; facial features, limbs, skin, hair, etc. When a living thing contradicts or strays vastly from it’s expected appearance in an obvious way, our perception is okay because the distance between what it is, and what is isn’t, creates a solid boundary (a person with a hook in place of a missing hand… a child dressed up in adult clothing… a person wearing an animal mask). However, when something is either too subtle in it’s altercation, or overcompensates in trying to appear as something else (a realistic, flesh-colored, rubber artificial hand in place of a missing hand… a child suffering from Progeria dressed in colorful children’s clothes… a life-like human mask draped over the head of an animal, where you can still see it’s eyes), it falls into the realm of the “uncanny valley.” If something attempts to appear or mimic a human or other living creature but stops just short of achieving that goal, we tend to focus on minute flaws, almost subconsciously, and the category of “flaw” becomes our perception of the whole.
Although he didn’t invent the concept, the term “uncanny valley” was resurrected by Japanese roboticist Masahiro Mori in his 1970 essay “The Uncanny Valley” (translated from Japanese), where he described in detail the alienated reactions that humans have to robots or androids who look too human-like (artificial skin, hair, animatronic faces and limbs), as opposed to human reactions to robots that do not try to appear human at all, and posses no human-like features, which are often instantly positive, empathetic and warm. Mori’s theories have become more and more important as robot science and robotic engineering have become acceleratingly widespread, particularly in Japan.
Mori actually commandeered the term from Ernst Jentsch, who coined it in his 1906 essay “On the Psychology of the Uncanny” (‘Ãœber die Psychologie des Unheimlichen’), which explored the thought processes humans go through within the boundary line that divides the familiar and the unfamiliar. These ideas were more famously expanded upon by Sigmund Freud, who wrote of them in his 1916 essay “The Uncanny” (‘Das Unheimliche’).
Of course, these days, things are a bit different. Once exposed to the uncanny, it eventually ceases to be so. And in our age of overly-developed communication technology and sensory exposure and overload, the instance of truly contradicting semiotics seems less and less likely. Today, fewer things seem genuinely “alien.”
Two years ago, while doing research for an article I wrote about living with a Japanese home robot, I had a fascinating phone conversation with Sara Kiesler, Professor of Computer Science and Human-Computer Interaction at Carnegie Mellon. A bit of a celebrity within the field of robotics on this side of the globe, Kiesler has, amongst other things, spent years in research with this exact topic. She spoke about how human characteristics in robots can often have a simple communicative purpose. A robot who extends an arm-like appendage to hand you something acts as information, you know what it is trying to do. Robots with no human features at all (something that is just a sphere or cylinder on wheels, something with no head, or even a typical home computer design), that interact with people in human-like ways can be just as disturbing as robots that try too hard to appear human. But the territory of either-or is not so black and white. The simple addition of human features onto a robot that otherwise appears to be a machine, initially thought of as positive in the design of robots, can be disturbing for many. A very mechanical looking robot that has added-on a human hand-like extension on it’s arm, or spherical eyes with pupils that correlate to move in the direction of what the robot is looking at, or human-like lips that move as the robot speaks, can sometimes remind people of disfigurement, strange disease, or death… mostly on a subconscious level. Conceivably, the emotional state and the complex variety of stimulus that cause the “uncanny valley” could be traced back evolutionarily to animals who developed an acute sense to identify other living things that were slightly diseased or mutated from the norm, and needed to be avoided.
I find the “uncanny valley” one of my favourite topis… maybe because I animate.. myabe becuase I am facinated but how certain things creep people out!
I’m interested in your comment that “Mori actually commandeered the term from Ernst Jentsch, who coined it in his 1906 essay “On the Psychology of the Uncannyâ€? (’Über die Psychologie des Unheimlichen’)” – does the German version actually contain the phrase ‘the uncanny *valley*’? I’ve read the English tranlation, and while Jentsch does describe example of things that certainly remind me of the UV effect, the valley reference does not appear.
“Of course, these days, things are a bit different. Once exposed to the uncanny, it eventually ceases to be so.”
I’m currently researching the uncanny valley, and extinction of the effect is one of the aspects that I’m interested in. Purely anecdotal so far, but I found that after I’d spent a long time working with images of one face that had originally triggered a UV response (http://www.communistrobot.com/images/robotnews/ever-2-muse-9.jpg) I no longer found it creepy. However, after a similar period of working with this one, it refuses to be extinguished – http://www.ed.ams.eng.osaka-u.ac.jp/development/Humanoid/ReplieeR1/img/CIMG0008.jpg.